From: The Art of Play: Helping Adults Reclaim Imagination and Spontaneity, by Adam Blatner, M.D. & Allee Blatner.
(Published by Routledge, 1997, now out of
print and being revised into 3rd edition. ) Comments welcome: Email to
adam@blatner.com
Chapter 1: A PSYCHOLOGY AND PHILOSOPHY OF PLAY
Adam Blatner & Allee Blatner
Revised September 2, 2008 (To see other chapters, go to Table of Contents)
The value of imaginative, sociodramatic play may be understood more
fully in the context of the union of a holistic, integrative psychology
with a philosophy that focuses on the creative process as a central
element. The connecting link between the two is the phenomenon of
spontaneity, that particular quality of mind in nature which allows for
novelty and creativity to emerge.
A Developmental Theory of Play
We describe a number of psychological dynamics in terms of the roles
that are played. It’s a way of thinking that allows for the great
variety of aspects of life and of the personality. In the following
pages, we’ll present reasons for the importance of such phenomena as
excitement, playfulness, celebration, delight, and enjoyment through
creativity. These phenomena haven’t been given enough emphasis in our
psychologies and our culture, and need to be recognized as vital
channels of motivation, self-expression, socialization, and skill
building. (Actually, the movement towards positive psychology has
gained a good deal of attention since 1997, and this supports our
efforts; however, The Art of Play focuses this broader goal
more in the arenas of imaginative dramatic enactment as a recreational
form and conscious role playing as a component in everyday life.)
Working from the dramaturgical model that notes the usefulness of
viewing much of psycho-social life from the metaphor of “all the
world’s a stage,” the unit of this model is the role concept (Blatner,
2007). Another especially useful idea is to note that healthy
development involves a process of role expansion in many dimensions.
For example, in addition to (or perhaps instead of) the psychoanalytic
classification of psycho-sexual phases of development, we present
psycho-social phases. Instead of thinking of humans as going through
oral, anal, phallic, and oedipal stages, we would propose in addition
(or perhaps even instead) four other stages: the dermal, playful, useful, and cooperative
stages. (These vaguely correlate with the psycho-sexual stages in age,
but reflect a more multi-dimensional process of role expansion.) These
stages emphasize more interpersonal and collective dynamics. In
addition, they offer a balancing alternative to what we feel has become
a bias towards individualism in much of psychology (Wallach &
Wallach, 1983).
Looking at development from the viewpoint of health rather than
sickness also seems to make sense, because the themes they involve
actually continue! People don’t grow out of the need for touching,
holding, playing, finding their own way to feel useful and socially
connected.
The Dermal Phase
The ancient Greek word root for skin is “derma,” and babies need to
feel themselves held, cuddled, stroked, touched. They feel through
their skin and deep pressure receptors. This phase also recognizes
other touch-like sensations such as balance, heat, coolness, and so
forth.
The skin is the largest organ in the human body, and as the experiments
of Harlow with the baby monkeys in the 1960s demonstrated, physical
contact is more important than mere feeding. Being held, cuddled,
caressed, rocked, and other sensual experiences are essential for
healthy psychological development. Moreover, in comparison to the oral
experience of feeding, the skin offers as rich (if not richer) a source
of pleasurable social bonding, sensuality, and discovery (Field, 2001).
Looking at child development from this perspective acknowledges the
significant role of fathers, older siblings, and other members of the
intimate community in healthy maturation.
The Playful Phase
The next phase, the playful, overlaps with and emerges out of the
dermal, occupying around the fifth to the twenty-fourth month and
beyond. Infants then begin to integrate their own initiative with the
richness of human relationships. In the healthiest forms of this phase,
there are many opportunities for mutual adventures, balanced
experiences of activity and receptivity on both sides, introduction of
novelty and allowance of time for assimilation, variations of pacing
and intensity (Stern, 1985). Sensitivity, time, and empathy are
required on the part of the parent, and these pleasant interactions for
the infant become the basis for a deep reservoir of pleasure and
positive expectations, which in turn give the child strength in dealing
with the inevitable frustrations of the coming years. Moreover, play
extends the role repertoire to include explorations of song, funny
faces, wordplay, and other modes of aesthetic, not directly
utilitarian, behavior.
The playful phase corresponds to and reframes the implicit struggle so
often associated with the anal stage in analytic theory. From the
individualistic focus of a training metaphor in which children must
learn to frustrate and control themselves, our playful phase represents
the opportunity for cooperative socialization. By using age-specific
games that go beyond the physical issue, it introduces to the growing
child the idea that there are places for things: for shoes and books
and poops and pee. The child can enjoy the mastery in learning to put
things where they belong.
Another major category that emerges halfway through this phase is the
idea of play, deception, paradox. A baby begins to tease a little,
offering things and pulling away with a giggle. And responds with a
giggle to having others play this game gently with her. The awareness
of “not really” is understood, even if this understanding cannot be
clearly articulated. Interestingly, even now I feel that I can’t
completely explain the meaning of play—and others have had difficulty
also (Sutton-Smith, 1998). Like feelings of fairness, we know it,
and we know when it feels like it’s “not play”—but it’s hard to define
strictly.
Usefulness
Building upon a healthy playful foundation, the next phases continue to
emphasize agreeable socialization rather than individualistic
dominance. Extending the play of putting things in the right place, the
child becomes aware of participation with people and what people do.
Helping feed the pets, clean the table, or sweep the driveway can
become enjoyable games that include the feeling of helping, of
experiencing being appreciated and thanked. The analytic phallic "Look
at me" is incorporated into our "useful" phase and would be stated in a
more social context as "How can I help?" (The Montessori Schools
approach build on this dynamic.)
Ample opportunities still exist in this schema to validate
individuality, in the sense of being recognized for one's originality
or special blend of strengths and weaknesses. It is important to
differentiate this from an overemphasis on individualism, in which the
boundary of mutuality is broken and a person expects or gets special
recognition and privileges even at another's expense. We propose the
use of collective play experiences to heighten the healthy elements of
self-esteem without promoting subtle pathological narcissism.
Cooperative Stage
The next step, extending further the natural cognitive maturation and
socialization process, corresponds to the analytic “Oedipal” stage.
Between four and six, children begin to learn to play with more than
one other person at a time. The challenge involves joining a small
group without being caught up in alliances. The interpersonal dynamic
is often much more significant than the intrapsychic experiences
involving sexual imagery. This cooperative stage can develop quite
smoothly in situations where the previous experiences have been
satisfying and the group (or parents) are themselves friendly and
inclusive.
For instance, when Adam's son, David, was five, he addressed this stage
while the two of them were alone together by announcing to Adam, "I'm
going to marry Mommy." Adam answered with mild curiosity and
friendliness, "But what about me? I'm already married to Mommy." David
thought for a moment, then made a simple synthesis: "Well, I'll marry
you, too." Both of them started laughing, the laughter also reflecting
David's partial yet implicit knowledge that one can't marry more than
one person at a time. As shall be discussed in the next chapter, the
playful capacity to celebrate paradox allows for a symbolic yet
satisfactory solution to many problems.
A psychology which views human nature as being essentially collective,
as well as individual, is an important tool in learning to think in
terms of how we can contribute to preventing or resolving a number of
our contemporary personal and societal problems (Blatner, 2004).
Another advantage to the role concept as a basis for a modern
psychology is its implicit invitation to become creative and flexible
with the roles we use in our lives. A great deal of healthy functioning
can be developed through practice in responsibly shifting roles. Play's
natural expansion of the range of roles we have available serves to
broaden and lubricate the process of human interaction.
Spontaneity
One of the pivotal concepts in relation to play is that of spontaneity.
The concept of spontaneity is a bit elusive because it involves the
process of transcending structure itself—the process of questioning
definitions and making redefinitions (Blatner, 2000, pp. 80-87). In
that sense, spontaneity partakes of the paradoxical nature of play (and this paradoxical quality is described
in Chapter 2.)
If you find yourself wondering if you're spontaneous enough to explore
improvisational playfulness, be assured that (a) the principles in this
book are designed so that you need not feel self-conscious, (b) you'll
discover that you've got more access to spontaneity than you might have
thought, and (c) your ability to channel spontaneity will develop with
practice, just as in other areas requiring cultivation of skills.
Everyone has some experience with spontaneity, and it varies in each
role and activity. You can learn to expand the function of this process
in your life.
Spontaneity involves a quality of mind, the active opening up that
accompanies the thinking of a new idea or trying something way. It
involves thinking afresh,
balancing impulse and restraint, and integrating imagination, reason,
and intuition, Spontaneity is the process by which inspiration
engenders creativity. It is more than mere impulsivity because it
requires some
intention to achieve an aesthetic or constructive effect.
Spontaneity also may be understood as the opposite of habit,
stereotyped thinking, neurotic compulsive rituals, or transferences
(interpersonal patterns that mimic earlier experiences, rather than
inter-acting with people and events in the here and now). In being
spontaneous, you are open to how the present moment is different from
the past, and how the people around you might be able to offer new and
more rewarding experiences.
Obviously, and yet worth mentioning, spontaneity is fun. You may
discover, however, as you begin to explore sociodramatic play, that you
experience some vague anxieties, guilts, or other subtle discomforts.
This may be part of what many people have learned about distrusting the
pursuit of pleasure. Enjoying pleasure is often partially repressed and
somewhat disreputable in our culture. Some of the reasons for this are
noted in Chapters 10 through 12 and deserve to be thought about when
you decide to begin exploring imaginative play. When things are
repressed, they are placed out of reach of consciousness and cannot be
updated, refined; thus, they remain undifferentiated, understood at the
level of consciousness—often childish—when they were pushed out of
awareness. For example, the idea of fun is too often associated with
unwholesome types of enjoyment. Our culture tends to pander to the
stronger emotions of sex and aggression just as it panders to our taste
for stronger flavors of fat, sugar, and salt in junk food. But just as
there are wholesome types of food, so also are there wholesome ways to
have fun.
The feelings of discomfort associated with play and having fun are
further complicated by getting associated with irresponsible frivolity,
destructive behaviors, or self-indulgent hedonism. These are actually
maladaptive forms of enjoyment and are characterized by feelings of
pent-up anger, worthlessness, and rebellion, often in response to
oppressive or coercive situations. They are seen in the behaviors of
those adolescents who cannot find enjoyable and meaningful involvements
in their (compulsory) school, and have joined the epidemic of drug and
alcohol abuse (Wigginton, 1985). Thus, when they have time off, or when
they drop out, they engage in drug-related short-term attempts at
having "fun" which in the long run result in a vacuous boredom.
Therefore, in the presence of all these negative associations, we need
to remind ourselves of the large array of deeply enjoyable pleasures to
choose from. Activities that are richer in resources, more sustainable,
more responsible, and often more social include: the arts,
storytelling, having adventures, being useful in creating celebrations,
exploring/contemplating nature and spiritual experiences, trying out
the full range of sports and games, and informal role-playing
activities such as the Art of Play.
Having acknowledged potential obstacles resulting from your own
psychosocial inhibitions to play, let's remember that it is fun.
Indeed, that may be the simplest definition of play. You can celebrate
with delight the discoveries of new potentialities arising out of your
unconscious, the magic of allowing the inspiration of the Muses to act
through you, the joy of surprise with the events that occur in a
receptive, cocreative state of mind, and the deep sense of belonging
that occurs when you and others share the intimacy of bringing forth
each other's images. Spontaneity then becomes a value as well as a
psychological phenomenon and, as such, deserves consideration from a
philosophical point of view.
Philosophical Reflections on Having Fun
A modern tradition in philosophy addresses itself to the implications
of the possibilities of evolution and the discoveries of modern
science. We refer, especially, to the writings of such philosophers as
Alfred North Whitehead (1938), Charles Hartshorne (1983), Pierre
Teilhard de Chardin (1965), and Ken Wilber (2007), among others. What
these thinkers share is a view of the universe as an essentially
creative process. Our human role in this universe is something similar
to the cells in the brain of an evolving, cosmic embryo. As humans
learn to relate to each other more harmoniously, it is as if we all are
participating in a great process of awakening. I discuss related themes in the 4th (revised) edition of Foundations of Psychodrama. (Blatner, 2000, pp. 66, 81).
Not only are our responsible acts of co-creation included in this
process, but also our experiences of learning and enjoyment. This
philosophical view is partly based on the validity of the aesthetic
component in events both great and small (Skutch, 1986). Another way of
saying this is that having fun may be a potentially important part of
the way everything operates in the universe.
In order to explore this concept, we invite you to consider the
following intuitive exposition, one which may be the way things are:
Imagine that pleasure pervades the universe: Every action involves at
least a “drop” of experience. Although Whitehead applied this even to
the atomic level, we'll focus our illustrations on the realms of those
events that involve the generally recognized forms of life. For
example, the cricket's "singing" may be a deliciously sensual
experience for the creature as it rubs its legs together. One-celled
animals in the ocean may be savoring the taste of their food during the
brief moments of their ingesting process. And that food consists of
even more minute creatures, algae and bacteria, who may be enjoying the
sun's warmth as the energy catalyzes their various internal chemical
processes.
Purely rational, dry "explanations" of animal behavior shouldn't
prevent us from contemplating the possible feelings generated in the
course of something being alive. This is not meant to imply animals
experience pleasure in the same way humans do, as commonly seen in the
anthropomorphic characters in children's stories. Nonetheless il seems
presumptuous for us to claim that they don't experience anything,
considering the complexity of their behavior. Indeed, we suspect that
in some ways human experiences and consciousness are a wonderful
complexification of a spectrum of inherent feelings in all life.
If hunger and fear "motivate" certain behaviors, then consider the
likelihood of feelings, however brief, when a creature catches its
prey, or takes the first bite, or, on the other hand, senses that it
has successfully evaded and escaped its potential predator. There is
probably a gradient of feeling, a sense of greater dissatisfaction,
leading to behavior, leading to a sense of relative satisfaction or
pleasure even in the most rudimentary functions of excreting,
discovering a richer, more oxygenated environment, or shifting from
rest to activity or back again.
The mystery of sex and parenting involves subtle components that
deserve contemplation. Beyond the culmination of orgasm, the mating
behavior of most animals involves discrimination, preference, and
choosing some experiences over others because they appear to offer more
fun to the participants. Attraction, courting, nest building, the
rearing of offspring, all are enormously complex phenomena which also
undoubtedly involve pleasure. Other examples include the singing of
many species of birds (Hartshorne, 1992), the strategies and chasing
behaviors of some predators, and the building of insect colonies.
A study of biology from the point of view of considering the personal
experience of the living creatures themselves can lead to a vivid sense
of the aesthetic dimensions pervading existence. In turn, it suggests
that the motivation of children and adults toward pleasure is a natural
phenomenon. Based on a holistic perspective, the philosophical
implications of a living universe which has vivid experiences invites
the integration of fun as an important value. Recognizing and honoring
fun from these perspectives sets the tone for the rest of our book.
References
Blatner, Adam. (2000). Foundations of psychodrama (4th ed.). New York: Springer.
Blatner, Adam. (2004). The collective in psychology. Retrieved from webpage: http://www.blatner.com/adam/psyntbk/collectv.htm
Blatner, A. (2007). Role dynamics: An integrative approach to
psychology and user-friendly language. Retrieved from
Webpage: http://www.blatner.com/adam/level2/roletheory.htm
Also: That article
links to a bibliography on applied role theory; and there are other
papers related to role theory, role analysis, the meta-role, at the
www.blatner.com/adam/papers.html website. In the
literature, see: Blatner, A. (1991). Role dynamics: A
comprehensive theory of psychology. Journal of Group Psychotherapy,
Psychodrama & Sociometry, 44 (1), 33-40.
Field, Tiffany. (2003). Touch. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hartshorne, Charles. (1983). Omnipotence and other theological mistakes. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Hartshorne, Charles. (1992). Born to sing: an interpretation and world
survey of bird song, Indiana Univ Press. (First published in 1973).
Skutch, A. F. (1986). Life ascending. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
Stern, Daniel. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.
Sutton-Smith, Brian. (2001). The ambiguity of play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Teilhard de Chardin, P. (1965). Hymn of the universe. New York: Harper Torchbooks, 1965.
Wallach, M. A., & Wallach, L. (1983). Psychology's sanction for
selfishness: The error of egoism in theory and therapy. San Francisco:
W.H. Freeman & Co.
Wigginton, E. (1985). Sometimes a shining moment: The foxfire
experience-Twenty years teaching in a high school classroom. Garden
City, NY: Anchor Press/Doubleday, 1985.
Whitehead, Alfred N. (1938). Modes of thought. New York: The Free Press.
Wilber, K. (2007). The integral vision: a very short introduction to
the revolutionary integral approach to life, god, the universe, and
everything Boston: Shalnbhala.
Some Further References on Play in Early Childhood. (There are
innumerable articles and books that have been written since the last
edition. Before that, though, here are a few.)
Christie, J. F. (Ed.). Play and early literacy development. Albany, NY: SUNY Press, 1991.
Dimidjian, V. J. (Ed.). Play's place in public education for young
children. Washington, DC: National EducationAssociation, 1992.
Goldstein, J. H. (Ed.). Toys, play and child development. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1994.
Isenberg, J. F, & Jalongo, M. R. Creative expression and play in
the early childhood curriculum. New York: Macmillan, 1993. (Chapter on
creative drama, pp. 134-170.)
Moyles, J. R. Just playing?: The role and status of play in early
childhood education. Philadelphia: Open University Press, 1989.
Singer, D. G., & Singer, J. L. The house of make believe: Play and
the developing imagination. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Universtiy Press,
1990.
Slade, A., & Wolf, D. P. (Eds.). Children at play: Clinical and
developmental approaches to meaning and representation. New York:
Oxford University Press, 1994. .(Many scholarly
references.)
Slade, P. Child play: Its importance for human development. London & Bristol, PA: Jessica Kingsley, 1995.